ABSTRACT

Given that behavioural disturbance is a

complex interplay between the patient,

symptoms, and response of those caring for

them there is unlikely to be a simple ‘cause’ of

behavioural disturbance. To take just one

example – aggression – understanding can be

sought at a neurobiological, clinical, or

environmental level. Thus specific neuronal

loss is reported in aggression in AD (Forstl et

al, 1994) and altered adrenergic function

could affect aggression; one study suggests

that alpha-2 adrenergic receptors are increased

in aggressive AD patients at post mortem

relative to patients without aggression (Russo-

Neustadt and Cotman, 1997). At a clinical

level, aggression has been shown to correlate

very highly with psychosis (Aarsland et al,

1996) and the presence of delusions are the

best clinical indicator for occurrence of

aggressive features (Gilley et al, 1997;

Gormley et al, 1998). However, it is also true

that aggression in patients is frequently in

response to some other event – a carer trying

to help the patient to dress or admission to a

new day-centre, for example. Similarly,

changes in levels of apathy or agitation may be

due to selective neuronal loss, to an

underlying depression, or to an external cause

such as a change in carer or physical illness

(Mintzer and Brawman Mintzer, 1996).