ABSTRACT

Traditional Approaches to Stress Research . . . . . . . . . 92

Theoretical Frameworks . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92

Appraisal Theory . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 92

Adaptation Under Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 93

The Cognitive-Energetic Framework . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 94

Relationship between Stress and

Cognitive Workload . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Cognitive Workload as a Form of Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Performance and Workload:

Associations/Dissociations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Mitigation of Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Changing the Person . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Training/skill development . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Personnel selection . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 96

Changing the Task . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97

Display design . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97

Adaptive automation . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 97

Hedonomics: Promoting Enjoyable

Human-Computer Interaction . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98

Teletic work and intrinsic motivation . . . . . . . . . . . . . 98

Intrinsic motivation and skill development . . . . . . . . 98

Teletic work and motivational affordances . . . . . . . . . 99

Problems for Future Research . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99

Understanding Mental Resources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99

Resource metaphors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 99

Function of resources . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100

Empirical tests of the model . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 100

Structural mechanisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101

Energetic mechanisms . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 101

Structure and energy . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102

Application of neuroergonomics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102

Development of the Adaptation under Stress Model . . . . . 102

Quantify the task dimensions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102

Attentional narrowing . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102

Stressor characteristics . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 102

Potential benefits of space-time distortion . . . . . . . . 103

Changes in adaptation: the roles

of time and intensity . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Understanding Performance-Workload

Associations/Dissociations . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Task factors . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Multidimensionality of workload . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Individual Differences in Performance,

Workload, and Stress . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Trait Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 103

Affective State Differences . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 104

Attentional narrowing and adaptive response . . . . . 105

Hedonomics and individual differences . . . . . . . . . . 105

Implications of Stress for Researchers

and Practitioners . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105

Summary and Conclusions . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 105

References . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . . 106

For those whose professional lives revolve around humancomputer interaction (HCI), they may ask themselves why they should even glance at a chapter on stress. While it is evident that many computer systems have to support people operating in stressful circumstances, there are important design issues concerning how to present information in these very demanding circumstances. However, one could still ask, are these of central interest to those in the mainstream of HCI? Indeed, if these were the only issues, we would agree and would recommend the reader to pass quickly onto something of much more evident relevance. However, we hope to persuade you that the relevance of stress research to HCI is not limited to such concerns. Indeed, we hope to convince the reader that stress, in the form of task loading, is central to all HCI. To achieve this, we first present a perspective that puts stress front and center in the HCI realm. Traditionally, stress has been seen as exposure to some adverse environmental circumstances, such as excessive heat, cold, noise, vibration, and so forth, and its effects manifest themselves primarily in relation to the physiological system most perturbed by the stress at hand. However, Hancock and Warm (1989) observed that stress effects are virtually all mediated through the brain, but for the cortex such effects are almost always of secondary concern since the brain is primarily involved with the goals of ongoing behavior or, more simply, the current task. Therefore, we want to change the orientation of concern so that stress is not just a result of peripheral interference but rather that the primary source of stress comes from the ongoing task itself. If we now see the task itself as the primary driving influence then stress concerns are central to all HCI issues.