ABSTRACT

Manipulation of three-dimensional objects affords children learning opportunities that are

less available in situations that involve purely verbal or written communication (Rogoff,

1990). As discussed in other chapters in this volume, and in the education literature more

broadly, the opportunity to touch and interact with objects is often very helpful for young

children as they attempt to understand abstract concepts or processes. Inspired by Piaget’s

theory (cf. Piaget & Inhelder, 1969), psychologists and education researchers have

posited various concrete-to-abstract shifts in children’s thinking. For example, whereas

preschool-aged children understand perceptually based analogies, older children

understand analogies based on more abstract relational features (Kotovsky & Gentner,

1996). Educational methods have often been developed with such concrete-abstract shifts

in mind. The manipulative materials used in early mathematics classrooms, for example,

have long been considered essential aids in communicating abstract principles to young

children. The usefulness of concrete objects as tools for abstract thought is not surprising,

given young children’s connection to the sensory reality of the here-and-now. In this

chapter, however, we focus on a growing view that concrete objects do not always

improve children’s understanding of abstract ideas, and, in fact, that the sensory

properties of certain objects, “representational objects,” can even pose an obstacle to

learning (Uttal, Liu, & DeLoache, 1999).