Skip to main content
Taylor & Francis Group Logo
Advanced Search

Click here to search books using title name,author name and keywords.

  • Login
  • Hi, User  
    • Your Account
    • Logout
Advanced Search

Click here to search books using title name,author name and keywords.

Breadcrumbs Section. Click here to navigate to respective pages.

Chapter

Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error

Chapter

Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error

DOI link for Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error

Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error book

Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error

DOI link for Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error

Irrational Numbers: Quantifying Accuracy and Error book

Edited ByJoachim I. Krueger
BookSocial Judgment and Decision Making

Click here to navigate to parent product.

Edition 1st Edition
First Published 2011
Imprint Psychology Press
Pages 18
eBook ISBN 9780203854150

ABSTRACT

E xperimental psychologists thrive on deciencies. Consider just a handful of shortcomings that interest them: judgmental overcondence, the bet-ter-than-average effect, correspondence bias, the fundamental attribution error, conrmation bias, the planning fallacy, gambler’s fallacy, hindsight bias, egocentric bias, the sunk-costs fallacy, the illusion of control, omission bias, neglect bias, and on and on (see Jussim, Stevens, & Salib, this volume, Chapter 6; Krueger, this volume, Chapter 4). These phenomena differ from one another in substantive regards, but they share an important quality. Each points to ways in which social judgments can be systematically distorted away from a criterion of “accuracy” or “rationality” that is held up as an ideal in a theoretical model of human cognition. Such comparisons between actual judgments and idealized judgments pervade the psychological literature. At times, the idealized model has been explicated as the response that should be observed when people adhere to rational thought processes (for example, Jones & Davis, 1965; Kelley, 1967). Other times, it is adapted from statistical or mathematical models (for example, Tversky & Kahneman, 1971). Still other times, the ideal is so obviously the “correct” response that researchers merely appeal to the counterintuitive nature of a response (Prentice & Miller, 1992).

T&F logoTaylor & Francis Group logo
  • Policies
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms & Conditions
    • Cookie Policy
    • Privacy Policy
    • Terms & Conditions
    • Cookie Policy
  • Journals
    • Taylor & Francis Online
    • CogentOA
    • Taylor & Francis Online
    • CogentOA
  • Corporate
    • Taylor & Francis Group
    • Taylor & Francis Group
    • Taylor & Francis Group
    • Taylor & Francis Group
  • Help & Contact
    • Students/Researchers
    • Librarians/Institutions
    • Students/Researchers
    • Librarians/Institutions
  • Connect with us

Connect with us

Registered in England & Wales No. 3099067
5 Howick Place | London | SW1P 1WG © 2021 Informa UK Limited